00:01 - 00:03

♪ ♪

00:06 - 00:10

SUSANA MARTINEZ-CONDE: The brain is the biggest mystery in science today.

00:10 - 00:14

THALIA WHEATLEY: It's responsible for all the facets of our personality,

00:14 - 00:16

everything we think and everything we feel.

00:16 - 00:18

It makes you you.

00:18 - 00:20

URI MAOZ: A very large fraction

00:20 - 00:24

of what's happening in my brain I am not aware of at all.

00:24 - 00:27

HEATHER BERLIN: But what exactly is going on in your unconscious brain?

00:27 - 00:30

What part of your brain is really in charge?

00:30 - 00:32

CHARLES LIMB: All day long, we're doing

00:32 - 00:34

unscripted things that we didn't know we would be doing.

00:34 - 00:35

(sizzling)

00:35 - 00:36

Life is not scripted.

00:37 - 00:40

MAOZ: Find a word that has some meaning for you.

00:40 - 00:42

BERLIN: So you might think you've made a choice...

00:42 - 00:44

Representation.

00:44 - 00:46

BERLIN: But in the back of your mind, you wonder...

00:46 - 00:48

Come on!

00:48 - 00:49

BERLIN: Was that really me?

00:49 - 00:51

♪ ♪

00:51 - 00:54

We might feel like we're in control.

00:54 - 00:56

ANIL SETH: This idea that we're in control of our actions

00:56 - 00:58

seems critical to our sense of identity.

00:59 - 01:02

BERLIN: But our brains may have other ideas.

01:02 - 01:05

BOBBY KASTHURI: The brain is made of almost 90 billion neurons,

01:05 - 01:07

but it produces this illusion

01:07 - 01:11

that there's a single person inside our skulls.

01:11 - 01:13

LUKE CHANG: For every Pinocchio, there's always someone

01:13 - 01:15

kind of pulling the strings behind the scenes.

01:15 - 01:16

(device beeps)

01:16 - 01:18

♪ ♪

01:18 - 01:22

MICHAEL GAZZANIGA: There can be two separated minds inside one system.

01:22 - 01:24

WHEATLEY: It's not just that motor,

01:24 - 01:26

memory, language is in the brain.

01:26 - 01:27

Your personality is up there,

01:27 - 01:30

your morality is up there.

01:30 - 01:34

BIANCA JONES MARLIN: We as humans know how environment and traumatic events

01:34 - 01:36

change people.

01:36 - 01:39

BERLIN: "Your Brain: Who's In Control?"

01:39 - 01:42

Right now, on "NOVA."

01:42 - 01:47

♪ ♪

01:55 - 01:57

BERLIN: Have you ever thought that you've made

01:57 - 01:59

a crystal-clear decision?

01:59 - 02:01

(inner voice): I'm just gonna watch two episodes tonight.

02:01 - 02:03

(narration): But the next thing you know...

02:04 - 02:05

(inner voice): Okay, just one more episode.

02:05 - 02:08

(laugh track playing)

02:08 - 02:09

Actually, it's time to go to bed.

02:09 - 02:11

♪ ♪

02:11 - 02:13

Well, I bet everyone else has already finished this season.

02:13 - 02:14

(static hissing, laugh track plays)

02:14 - 02:16

Wait, why am I still watching this?

02:16 - 02:19

(narration): Well, of course, the answer lies in

02:19 - 02:21

your brain.

02:21 - 02:22

♪ ♪

02:22 - 02:25

Your brain contains multitudes.

02:25 - 02:30

It's a complex and intricate three-pound piece of matter.

02:30 - 02:32

But you actually have no awareness

02:32 - 02:33

of most of the things that are going on

02:33 - 02:35

inside your brain.

02:35 - 02:40

I'm neuroscientist and clinical psychologist Heather Berlin.

02:40 - 02:43

(laughs)

02:40 - 02:43

Come on, man!

02:43 - 02:45

BERLIN: And I'm on a journey to discover

02:46 - 02:48

what's really driving the decisions you make?

02:48 - 02:49

(device clicks)

02:49 - 02:51

No agency at all.

02:52 - 02:54

Who or what is really in control?

02:55 - 02:58

There are important unconscious processes

02:58 - 02:59

in your brain that you're not aware of.

02:59 - 03:02

Most of the time, the brain is a coordinated,

03:02 - 03:05

well-oiled machine, with different brain regions

03:05 - 03:08

working together in harmony.

03:08 - 03:10

(audio distorting): But under certain circumstances,

03:10 - 03:11

when things are out of sync,

03:11 - 03:13

we can gain deeper insight

03:13 - 03:17

into how the brain actually works.

03:17 - 03:20

(wildlife chirping)

03:20 - 03:26

♪ ♪

03:28 - 03:29

There's one thing we do every day

03:29 - 03:31

with little to no conscious control.

03:31 - 03:35

It's something you might spend a whole third of your life doing:

03:35 - 03:36

sleeping.

03:36 - 03:41

When we sleep, we're supposed to be unconscious and at rest.

03:41 - 03:46

But for some people, that's not always the case.

03:46 - 03:50

♪ ♪

03:50 - 03:52

(mumbling)

03:52 - 03:54

BERLIN: These are people who sleepwalk.

03:54 - 03:56

MAN: Just like, just like you were?

03:56 - 03:57

EMMANUEL DURING: Sleepwalking is a glitch

03:57 - 03:59

in the system,

03:59 - 04:01

because our identity is not in control.

04:01 - 04:05

And that's what a lot of my patients

04:05 - 04:07

tell me, like, they, "I didn't do that.

04:07 - 04:10

"That's not possible.

04:10 - 04:11

This is not me."

04:11 - 04:17

♪ ♪

04:19 - 04:20

So, sleepwalking?

04:20 - 04:22

Very common condition or phenomenon.

04:22 - 04:24

Simply said, it's what the word is.

04:24 - 04:26

You sleep,

04:26 - 04:29

but during your sleep, you will walk.

04:29 - 04:31

We take it for granted, right?

04:31 - 04:32

But the walking is extremely complex.

04:32 - 04:35

Just teaching a robot

04:35 - 04:39

all the inputs and outputs for a body

04:39 - 04:44

to move forward on two legs without falling.

04:44 - 04:47

All of this, you don't even think about it.

04:47 - 04:50

It works independently.

04:50 - 04:52

♪ ♪

04:52 - 04:55

BERLIN: How is it possible to do complex behaviors like walking,

04:55 - 05:00

eating, and sometimes even driving while sleeping?

05:00 - 05:01

♪ ♪

05:01 - 05:03

To find out, I'm visiting a sleep center

05:03 - 05:06

at the Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai.

05:06 - 05:08

So, tell me a little bit

05:08 - 05:10

about what's happening with you at night

05:10 - 05:12

and your sleepwalking.

05:12 - 05:14

Well, I've been doing some weird things.

05:15 - 05:16

I painted a wall in my living room

05:16 - 05:18

and one in my kitchen.

05:18 - 05:20

I made a triangle, a perfect triangle...

05:20 - 05:21

What?

05:20 - 05:21

...in my kitchen.

05:21 - 05:22

So what do you think

05:22 - 05:23

when you find that?

05:24 - 05:25

Like...

05:24 - 05:25

I don't know.

05:25 - 05:26

Just-- I laugh, because I go,

05:26 - 05:28

"How the heck I did this?"

05:29 - 05:32

BERLIN: Emmanuel During studies what's going on in the brain

05:32 - 05:33

when someone sleepwalks.

05:33 - 05:36

In the center, sleep patients are wired up with sensors

05:36 - 05:39

that pick up eye and body movements--

05:39 - 05:42

as well as their brain waves-- while they sleep.

05:43 - 05:44

So what are we looking at here,

05:44 - 05:45

these blue lines?

05:45 - 05:47

DURING: These are the eye movements.

05:47 - 05:49

Okay, and then the, the black lines here?

05:49 - 05:50

These are the brain waves.

05:50 - 05:51

So this patient is obviously,

05:51 - 05:52

he's lying in bed.

05:52 - 05:53

Mm-hmm.

05:54 - 05:57

And dozes off slowly, feels sleepy.

05:57 - 05:59

And as we move on, he dives into

05:59 - 06:01

deep slow-wave sleep.

06:01 - 06:03

BERLIN: During sleep, your brain

06:03 - 06:07

cycles through phases of high and low activity.

06:07 - 06:09

When the brain waves slow down,

06:09 - 06:11

scientists call this "deep sleep."

06:11 - 06:14

But when someone sleepwalks...

06:14 - 06:17

First of all, everything looks good.

06:17 - 06:20

You see the brainwaves.

06:17 - 06:20

Mm-hmm.

06:20 - 06:22

Everything is very, very monotonous,

06:22 - 06:23

sort of slow waves.

06:22 - 06:23

Mm-hmm.

06:24 - 06:25

And then it's interesting,

06:25 - 06:27

since there's a buildup of slow wave,

06:27 - 06:29

that the amplitude goes up,

06:29 - 06:31

and then suddenly...

06:31 - 06:33

Whoa.

06:31 - 06:33

So he's seemingly awake.

06:33 - 06:34

Sudden.

06:33 - 06:34

Yeah.

06:34 - 06:37

Looks like a sudden arousal.

06:37 - 06:39

Looks sort of scared.

06:37 - 06:39

I mean, very brief.

06:39 - 06:40

Very fast, eyes open.

06:40 - 06:43

There's a, sort of a split, then.

06:43 - 06:46

BERLIN: The patient looks like they're awake.

06:46 - 06:50

But a couple of key brain regions seem to stay asleep.

06:50 - 06:53

DURING: There's part of the brain stays in slow-wave sleep.

06:53 - 06:56

It's such a deep stage of, of sleep, it's hard to wake up,

06:56 - 06:58

and the other part of the brain is already awake.

06:58 - 07:01

BERLIN: One part of the brain that doesn't wake up during

07:01 - 07:04

sleepwalking is called the prefrontal cortex.

07:04 - 07:06

It's the region of the brain responsible for

07:06 - 07:09

deliberate choices and self-awareness.

07:09 - 07:13

DURING: This prefrontal cortex is the decision maker.

07:13 - 07:15

The other areas of the brain

07:15 - 07:18

can mostly work independently of that.

07:18 - 07:20

♪ ♪

07:20 - 07:22

So, essentially, so many parts of the brain

07:22 - 07:26

can be engaged without conscious awareness of it.

07:26 - 07:29

BERLIN: During sleepwalking, the motor cortex,

07:29 - 07:32

which controls movement, the visual cortex,

07:32 - 07:34

which processes visual information,

07:34 - 07:35

and the parts of the brain

07:35 - 07:38

that coordinate behaviors like balance and speech

07:38 - 07:39

can all become active

07:39 - 07:42

without engaging the prefrontal cortex.

07:43 - 07:45

MAN: And what exactly are you doing, ma'am?

07:45 - 07:49

It's a special code.

07:49 - 07:53

MARTINEZ-CONDE: Experiences of sleepwalking reveal that being conscious

07:53 - 07:56

is not an all-or-none situation.

07:56 - 08:01

Our unconscious makes a lot of everyday decisions for us.

08:01 - 08:04

NANCY KANWISHER: For starters, boring stuff,

08:04 - 08:06

like regulating your heart rate and your temperature

08:06 - 08:08

and deciding when to take the food in your stomach

08:08 - 08:10

and move it down into your gut.

08:10 - 08:13

Like, thank God we don't have to be aware of all that stuff.

08:13 - 08:16

DANIELA SCHILLER: Motor function, sensory function,

08:16 - 08:17

motor-sensory integration,

08:17 - 08:19

memory representation.

08:19 - 08:20

All of this is happening

08:20 - 08:25

below the surface, like the inside of a clockwork.

08:25 - 08:28

(man mumbling)

08:25 - 08:28

BERLIN: When you sleepwalk,

08:29 - 08:31

the brain regions that control your movement, vision,

08:31 - 08:34

and breathing can get up to all kinds of mischief

08:34 - 08:36

without you even knowing it.

08:36 - 08:40

But there's one case where even those regions check out--

08:40 - 08:42

during anesthesia.

08:42 - 08:44

♪ ♪

08:44 - 08:45

We know that there are drugs

08:45 - 08:46

that I can give you, anesthetics,

08:46 - 08:49

that would remove your conscious experience.

08:50 - 08:53

SETH: And we all know that consciousness comes in degrees.

08:53 - 08:55

Like, we can lose consciousness in sleep,

08:55 - 08:57

but then we lose it in a more profound way

08:57 - 09:00

when we are under general anesthesia.

09:01 - 09:04

BERLIN: When I was a young researcher working in anesthesiology,

09:04 - 09:06

I saw this firsthand.

09:06 - 09:11

So what happens to your brain activity when you go under?

09:11 - 09:16

♪ ♪

09:17 - 09:19

Neuroscientist Emery Brown is measuring the line

09:19 - 09:21

that separates being conscious

09:21 - 09:24

from being unconscious.

09:24 - 09:26

BROWN: I want to guarantee my patients

09:26 - 09:27

that when I say you're unconscious,

09:27 - 09:29

you're not going to perceive pain,

09:29 - 09:31

you won't be moving around,

09:31 - 09:33

you won't remember anything that's occurring.

09:33 - 09:35

Your heart rate and blood pressure

09:35 - 09:36

and other physiological systems will be well-controlled.

09:36 - 09:41

♪ ♪

09:41 - 09:44

BERLIN: The patient is undergoing surgery.

09:44 - 09:46

But before the surgeons can operate,

09:46 - 09:48

the anesthesiologists have to put her under--

09:48 - 09:52

render her unconscious with special drugs.

09:52 - 09:53

WOMAN: I'm starting to give you medicines

09:53 - 09:55

that might make you feel kind of drowsy.

09:55 - 09:57

BROWN: Look straight ahead.

09:57 - 09:58

Look straight ahead.

09:58 - 10:00

See, her eyes move

10:00 - 10:01

as we expect them to move.

10:01 - 10:03

So you're moving her head, but her eyes stay straight.

10:03 - 10:05

WOMAN: All right, now we're going to have you breathe

10:05 - 10:07

a little oxygen.

10:05 - 10:07

BROWN: Breathe some oxygen for a minute.

10:07 - 10:09

And can you see my finger here?

10:09 - 10:12

Follow it with your eyes.

10:12 - 10:15

And if you can't follow it anymore, tell me, all right?

10:17 - 10:18

Can you hear me?

10:18 - 10:21

♪ ♪

10:21 - 10:23

See her eyes are fixed now?

10:21 - 10:23

BERLIN: Yeah.

10:23 - 10:26

You see the E.E.G. has a large, slow oscillation?

10:26 - 10:27

See that?

10:26 - 10:27

BERLIN: Yeah, yeah.

10:27 - 10:29

BROWN: Her brain stem is out.

10:29 - 10:31

BERLIN: It's out, that's it?

10:29 - 10:31

BROWN: Mm-hmm.

10:31 - 10:34

BERLIN: When you go under, it can feel like

10:34 - 10:36

one second you're here, and the next,

10:36 - 10:39

you're out.

10:39 - 10:42

What's going on in the brain when this happens?

10:42 - 10:46

Emery uses a device called an E.E.G.,

10:47 - 10:49

a set of electrodes that rests on the scalp

10:49 - 10:52

and detects electrical activity in the brain.

10:52 - 10:56

That activity comes in the form of waves.

10:56 - 11:00

BROWN: The brain generates brain waves or oscillations.

11:00 - 11:01

And there are oscillations

11:01 - 11:04

that we typically see when someone's conscious.

11:04 - 11:07

BERLIN: These brain waves are measured by their frequency,

11:07 - 11:09

how fast the waves come and go,

11:09 - 11:13

and by their amplitude, how small or big the waves are.

11:13 - 11:14

BROWN: I look at your E.E.G.

11:14 - 11:16

When you're awake,

11:16 - 11:18

you're going to have a very rich response.

11:18 - 11:21

When I anesthetize you, it goes away.

11:21 - 11:23

And so the difference between those two states

11:23 - 11:25

represents the transition from being conscious

11:25 - 11:27

to the unconscious.

11:27 - 11:28

See the oscillations, see how they're

11:28 - 11:29

really big now.

11:28 - 11:29

BERLIN: Yeah.

11:29 - 11:31

And before, see, they were just sort of little...

11:31 - 11:32

Yeah, exactly.

11:31 - 11:32

Kind of, yeah.

11:32 - 11:35

(talking in background)

11:35 - 11:37

BERLIN: When you're awake and fully aware,

11:37 - 11:41

your brain wave activity is diverse and dynamic.

11:41 - 11:44

It looks kind of like an exciting conversation.

11:44 - 11:49

But when anesthesia drugs hit the brain,

11:49 - 11:52

the activity is dramatically reduced

11:52 - 11:55

to dull, slow-rolling brain waves.

11:55 - 11:57

The once dynamic conversation

11:57 - 12:00

becomes an unintelligible hum.

12:00 - 12:01

BROWN: If you alter how

12:01 - 12:03

the parts of the brain communicate sufficiently,

12:03 - 12:04

you can make someone unconscious.

12:04 - 12:07

So that's what the drugs are doing.

12:07 - 12:08

They're altering the way

12:08 - 12:09

the various parts of the brain communicate.

12:11 - 12:15

BERLIN: There's one region of the brain in particular that acts as

12:15 - 12:18

a communication hub: the thalamus.

12:18 - 12:21

It's made up of two parts, each about the size of a walnut,

12:21 - 12:24

and sits deep inside your brain.

12:24 - 12:26

BROWN: Thalamus is a central way station

12:26 - 12:28

for all sorts of information processing.

12:28 - 12:30

Auditory information goes through there,

12:30 - 12:32

visual information goes through there,

12:32 - 12:35

pain information goes through there.

12:35 - 12:36

If I could take out

12:37 - 12:38

just one brain center to make you unconscious,

12:38 - 12:40

it would probably be the thalamus,

12:40 - 12:42

because it's such a central actor

12:42 - 12:44

in processing all types of information.

12:45 - 12:47

BERLIN: After a couple of hours of surgery,

12:47 - 12:51

the medical team is tapering off the anesthesia drugs.

12:51 - 12:56

And the E.E.G. reveals the patient's brain wave activity

12:56 - 12:58

becoming more complex as she wakes up.

12:58 - 13:00

ANTHONY: She's starting to take

13:00 - 13:01

some breaths on her own.

13:00 - 13:01

BROWN: Yeah.

13:01 - 13:03

ANTHONY: Open your eyes wide.

13:03 - 13:05

And squeeze my hand.

13:05 - 13:08

BROWN: Consciousness is really having active cognitive processing,

13:09 - 13:10

being able to think and act.

13:10 - 13:12

ANTHONY: Surgery's all done, okay?

13:12 - 13:13

BROWN: It's the integration of that information

13:13 - 13:16

which allows us to start to understand

13:16 - 13:18

how consciousness is actually formed.

13:21 - 13:22

♪ ♪

13:22 - 13:24

KASTHURI: Consciousness can obviously

13:24 - 13:26

interact with the physical world like we can.

13:26 - 13:28

We can use drugs to remove it.

13:28 - 13:31

We go to sleep and we're not conscious, and yet,

13:31 - 13:33

it's tenuous at the same time.

13:33 - 13:36

We can't say how any specific set of neurons

13:36 - 13:39

working together produces consciousness.

13:39 - 13:41

REBECCA SAXE: It's so clear that anesthesia

13:41 - 13:44

is some kind of change of consciousness, right?

13:44 - 13:48

The whole brain is there, the pieces are there,

13:48 - 13:50

but the messages aren't getting through

13:50 - 13:52

in a way that makes for our conscious experience.

13:52 - 13:53

(static hissing, beeps distorting)

13:53 - 13:55

And that's the difference between

13:55 - 13:57

being aware and not being aware.

13:58 - 14:00

BERLIN: So the level of communication

14:00 - 14:02

among brain regions is one difference

14:02 - 14:05

between being conscious and being unconscious.

14:05 - 14:10

That means that no single area of the brain

14:10 - 14:13

is responsible for your consciousness.

14:13 - 14:17

It's that communication that helps make you you.

14:17 - 14:20

MAN: Now, let's remember that the left hand

14:20 - 14:22

is governed from the right hemisphere.

14:22 - 14:25

BERLIN: For some people, an entire half of their brain

14:25 - 14:28

can't really communicate with the rest.

14:28 - 14:32

These are people who have undergone split-brain surgery,

14:32 - 14:34

and it's as if...

14:34 - 14:38

(audio doubled): They have two minds in a single brain.

14:38 - 14:39

MAN: Now the question becomes,

14:39 - 14:41

what happens when you allow

14:41 - 14:43

both hands together to try to solve the problem?

14:43 - 14:45

And what we find out is that they fight over each other.

14:45 - 14:48

One hand knows how to do it and one hand does not,

14:48 - 14:51

and so they more or less squabble.

14:51 - 14:53

The human brain contains two sides,

14:53 - 14:55

the left hemisphere and the right hemisphere, right?

14:55 - 14:58

And they are connected by a big bundle of fibers.

14:58 - 15:00

It's called the corpus callosum.

15:00 - 15:03

All the communication from one side of the brain

15:03 - 15:07

to the other has to pass through this fiber bundle.

15:07 - 15:11

BERLIN: For some people with epilepsy, a seizure in one hemisphere

15:11 - 15:16

can quickly spread to the other by way of the corpus callosum.

15:16 - 15:18

But if that bridge is surgically severed,

15:19 - 15:22

a seizure can no longer cross to the other side of the brain.

15:23 - 15:25

In addition to treating epilepsy,

15:25 - 15:27

these surgeries have also led

15:27 - 15:29

to some astounding research into

15:29 - 15:32

how the two hemispheres function.

15:32 - 15:35

MILLER: With your left hand, make me the a-okay sign.

15:35 - 15:36

(woman laughs)

15:36 - 15:40

BERLIN: To learn more about these fascinating studies,

15:40 - 15:42

I met two pioneers in the field:

15:42 - 15:44

Michael Miller

15:44 - 15:46

and Michael Gazzaniga.

15:46 - 15:49

Michael Miller asked me

15:49 - 15:52

to step into his lab to do a few simple tests,

15:52 - 15:55

just like the ones he's conducted with patients

15:55 - 15:56

after split-brain surgery.

15:56 - 15:58

So, Heather, what you're going to see

15:58 - 15:59

are two shapes.

15:59 - 16:01

They're going to come up on the screen.

16:01 - 16:02

♪ ♪

16:03 - 16:05

You're gonna draw the shape on the left side of the screen

16:05 - 16:06

with your left hand,

16:06 - 16:07

and the shape on the right side of the screen

16:07 - 16:09

with your right hand.

16:09 - 16:11

And I want you to draw them as quickly as you can

16:11 - 16:13

at the same time.

16:13 - 16:14

Okay?

16:13 - 16:14

(laughs): Okay.

16:14 - 16:16

BERLIN: Piece of cake, right?

16:16 - 16:18

(device beeps)

16:19 - 16:20

Oh...

16:19 - 16:20

MILLER: Beautiful.

16:20 - 16:21

(laughs)

16:24 - 16:25

Okay, not sure what you were drawing over here, but...

16:25 - 16:27

(laughs)

16:27 - 16:28

(device beeps)

16:29 - 16:31

Oh. (chuckles)

16:29 - 16:31

(laughing): Okay.

16:31 - 16:32

Did I mention I didn't get that much sleep last night?

16:32 - 16:33

(laughs)

16:35 - 16:37

BERLIN: The left side of the brain

16:37 - 16:39

controls most of the right side of the body.

16:39 - 16:41

And the right side of the brain

16:41 - 16:43

controls most of the left side of the body.

16:45 - 16:46

(all laughing)

16:46 - 16:48

What happened is, I started out trying to do different things,

16:48 - 16:50

and then they just sort of, like, sync up together.

16:48 - 16:50

MILLER: Yeah, yeah.

16:50 - 16:52

♪ ♪

16:52 - 16:53

(laughs)

16:52 - 16:53

Come on, man.

16:53 - 16:55

MILLER: It's perfectly normal.

16:55 - 16:58

So, I mean, what's happening is that the motor commands

16:58 - 17:00

in the, in one hemisphere...

16:58 - 17:00

Right.

17:00 - 17:01

...are interfering with the motor commands

17:01 - 17:03

in the other hemisphere.

17:03 - 17:06

BERLIN: It was basically impossible

17:06 - 17:07

for me to force my hands

17:07 - 17:10

to draw two different things at the same time.

17:12 - 17:13

But for someone whose two hemispheres

17:13 - 17:17

are disconnected, there's no interference.

17:17 - 17:19

It's almost as if there's one mind

17:19 - 17:21

controlling the left hand,

17:21 - 17:24

and a completely different mind controlling the right hand.

17:24 - 17:26

And it isn't just movement

17:26 - 17:28

that's split across the hemispheres.

17:28 - 17:33

Only half of your visual field goes to each side of the brain.

17:33 - 17:35

MILLER: When you're looking straight ahead,

17:35 - 17:38

everything to the left side of that space

17:38 - 17:41

goes only to the right hemisphere.

17:41 - 17:44

And the opposite is true for the right side of the space.

17:44 - 17:48

GAZZANIGA: The left part of the brain is where your language

17:48 - 17:49

and speech centers are.

17:49 - 17:51

That enables you to talk,

17:51 - 17:54

enables you to understand language, and all the rest.

17:54 - 17:55

And the right side of your brain

17:56 - 17:59

is very important in the evaluation of emotions,

17:59 - 18:03

evaluation of visual space.

18:03 - 18:05

I'm going to give you a test.

18:05 - 18:07

MAN: If you look right at my nose,

18:07 - 18:09

I'm going to hold up my hands.

18:09 - 18:11

You tell me how many fingers you see, all right?

18:11 - 18:13

GAZZANIGA: How many fingers do you see?

18:13 - 18:15

You see two, right?

18:15 - 18:17

Why did you see two?

18:17 - 18:21

(chuckling): This one went to your left hemisphere,

18:21 - 18:23

this one went to your right hemisphere,

18:23 - 18:25

way over in the other side of your brain.

18:25 - 18:27

How does your left hemisphere know about it?

18:27 - 18:30

That pathway, the corpus callosum.

18:30 - 18:32

It transfers that information.

18:32 - 18:35

Now I'm going to split your brain,

18:35 - 18:36

and I do the same test.

18:36 - 18:39

How many fingers do I see?

18:40 - 18:41

WOMAN: Two.

18:41 - 18:43

You see anything else?

18:45 - 18:46

No.

18:46 - 18:48

Okay.

18:48 - 18:50

You see one, you see this one,

18:50 - 18:54

because that goes straight to your left, talking hemisphere.

18:54 - 18:57

This one is still going to your right hemisphere,

18:57 - 19:01

which has now been disconnected from your left.

19:01 - 19:04

So your left brain can't talk about this.

19:04 - 19:06

So you now say you only see one finger,

19:06 - 19:10

even though your right brain is seeing this finger.

19:10 - 19:13

It just can't talk about it,

19:13 - 19:16

because the highway that communicates that information

19:16 - 19:17

has been cut.

19:17 - 19:20

Show me with your right hand what you see.

19:20 - 19:22

Two.

19:20 - 19:22

Okay.

19:22 - 19:25

Put it down, relax.

19:25 - 19:28

Show me with your left hand what you see.

19:29 - 19:31

One.

19:29 - 19:31

Good.

19:31 - 19:34

MILLER: It's the most remarkable thing to witness.

19:34 - 19:36

You know, there's this whole other entity

19:36 - 19:39

in the head that's controlling the body

19:39 - 19:41

and can understand and remember

19:41 - 19:43

and feel and think all on its own,

19:43 - 19:45

completely separate from the other side.

19:47 - 19:49

BERLIN: The researchers conducted tests

19:49 - 19:51

to explore how a split-brain patient's

19:52 - 19:54

two hemispheres work independently

19:54 - 19:56

from one another--

19:56 - 19:58

including a now-famous experiment

19:58 - 19:59

of a patient named Joe.

19:59 - 20:02

GAZZANIGA: Look right at the dot.

20:02 - 20:04

BERLIN: By quickly flashing a word

20:04 - 20:06

to just the left side of his visual field...

20:06 - 20:08

(device beeps)

20:06 - 20:08

GAZZANIGA: See anything?

20:08 - 20:09

BERLIN: ...that word would go exclusively

20:09 - 20:11

to the right half of his brain,

20:11 - 20:14

the half that can't talk.

20:14 - 20:16

So the only way we're going to know that it registered

20:16 - 20:19

is if he can write something out, okay?

20:19 - 20:22

With his hand that is controlled by his right hemisphere.

20:22 - 20:24

Exactly, his left hand.

20:22 - 20:24

The left hand.

20:24 - 20:26

GAZZANIGA: We flash the word "Texas."

20:26 - 20:28

GAZZANIGA: Look right at the dot.

20:28 - 20:29

See anything?

20:29 - 20:31

There's a flash.

20:29 - 20:31

All right.

20:31 - 20:33

I didn't see the word.

20:33 - 20:36

His right hemisphere is seeing it.

20:36 - 20:38

GAZZANIGA: We're seeing it, but the right hemisphere,

20:38 - 20:41

at this point in his surgery,

20:41 - 20:43

cannot talk.

20:41 - 20:43

Right.

20:43 - 20:49

GAZZANIGA: All right, I want you to draw for me that thing

20:49 - 20:51

upside down.

20:51 - 20:53

BERLIN: So he claims to not have seen anything.

20:54 - 20:56

Yeah.

20:54 - 20:56

Oh, my God.

20:56 - 20:57

(laughs)

20:56 - 20:57

Wow.

20:57 - 21:00

BERLIN: He was able to do Texas upside down.

20:57 - 21:00

GAZZANIGA: Yeah.

21:00 - 21:02

MILLER: But what's interesting is, he had no idea what he's drawing.

21:03 - 21:04

MILLER: We know because we saw the word.

21:04 - 21:06

JOE (chuckling): I can't tell what it is.

21:06 - 21:08

BERLIN: Wow.

21:08 - 21:10

GAZZANIGA: So then, later on, I show him the word again

21:10 - 21:11

and I ask a different question

21:11 - 21:14

about what he saw.

21:14 - 21:16

BERLIN: Once again, they showed the word "Texas"

21:16 - 21:19

to just his right, non-verbal hemisphere.

21:19 - 21:21

So when asked about what he saw,

21:21 - 21:24

all his left hemisphere can say is...

21:24 - 21:27

I'm aware of a word, I just didn't see what it was.

21:27 - 21:30

GAZZANIGA (in video): Draw something that goes with that.

21:30 - 21:32

A symbol of that.

21:34 - 21:36

BERLIN: Oh, wow,

21:36 - 21:37

so he draws a cowboy hat.

21:36 - 21:37

MILLER: Yeah, clearly...

21:37 - 21:38

Yeah, clearly, his right hemisphere

21:38 - 21:40

knows exactly what he's drawing.

21:38 - 21:40

Wow.

21:40 - 21:43

But his left is still confused,

21:43 - 21:44

so he doesn't understand it.

21:43 - 21:44

Right.

21:44 - 21:46

GAZZANIGA: What's that?

21:46 - 21:47

Cowboy hat.

21:48 - 21:49

Cowboy hat?

21:49 - 21:51

What was the word?

21:51 - 21:52

(whispering): So amazing.

21:51 - 21:52

JOE (in video): Texas.

21:52 - 21:53

(laughing): I can't believe it.

21:54 - 21:57

GAZZANIGA: Did you see "Texas"?

21:54 - 21:57

No.

21:57 - 22:01

GAZZANIGA: The split-brain phenomenon suggests that there can be

22:01 - 22:03

two separated minds, if you will,

22:03 - 22:05

inside of a skull.

22:05 - 22:09

The cooperation is on the paper, not inside the head.

22:09 - 22:11

It's an astounding example

22:11 - 22:15

of cross-cueing and management of two mental systems

22:15 - 22:16

into one unified act.

22:17 - 22:19

And the idea is

22:19 - 22:22

maybe that's going on in us all the time, too.

22:24 - 22:27

KANWISHER: Each of us has a sense that we're a unitary being,

22:27 - 22:29

but actually, that belies the fact

22:29 - 22:31

that each of us, each of our minds,

22:31 - 22:33

is actually composed of lots of different pieces

22:33 - 22:34

that are doing different things.

22:34 - 22:36

And different information can be represented

22:36 - 22:39

in different parts of that machinery.

22:39 - 22:43

And so a search for "where am I in all of this?"

22:43 - 22:44

is a little bit misguided,

22:44 - 22:46

because the "I" is not

22:46 - 22:48

such a unitary thing in the first place.

22:48 - 22:53

KASTHURI: That feeling of unity, of "me," is actually distributed

22:53 - 22:56

across almost 90 billion neurons.

22:56 - 23:00

This illusion that there's a single person

23:00 - 23:01

inside our skulls.

23:01 - 23:03

♪ ♪

23:03 - 23:07

BERLIN: Inside your brain are over 100 distinct regions.

23:07 - 23:09

Many different systems in the brain

23:09 - 23:11

control what you do, from movement,

23:11 - 23:15

to vision, to speech, and even social interaction.

23:17 - 23:19

MAHZARIN BANAJI: I think most human beings like to believe

23:19 - 23:22

that their mind is under their own control.

23:22 - 23:25

If I want to, I can stand up right now.

23:25 - 23:26

I can do that.

23:26 - 23:30

And that gives me, I think, the false belief

23:30 - 23:32

that everything I do has been chosen by me.

23:33 - 23:35

And if there is a story from the brain to tell,

23:35 - 23:37

it is that we are quite wrong.

23:37 - 23:39

BERLIN: Not only are there multiple parts

23:39 - 23:41

of your brain influencing you,

23:42 - 23:43

but there are things in the world around you

23:43 - 23:46

that influence your brain,

23:46 - 23:48

including other people.

23:48 - 23:51

SAXE: How we act and who we are in our lives

23:51 - 23:54

is hugely determined by

23:54 - 23:56

the expectations of the people around us.

23:56 - 24:00

The brain helps us be the most social species on the planet.

24:00 - 24:04

A lot of our brains are devoted to understanding other people.

24:04 - 24:08

SCHILLER: Our brain doesn't operate in isolation.

24:08 - 24:12

We constantly learn, take, compare to other brains.

24:12 - 24:15

CHANG: Our brains have evolved to be able to

24:15 - 24:18

effortlessly reason about other people.

24:18 - 24:20

And emotions, similarly, have evolved

24:20 - 24:22

as ways that guide our behavior.

24:22 - 24:26

BERLIN: So, how exactly do emotions--

24:26 - 24:30

and the emotions of others-- influence our brains?

24:31 - 24:33

Neuroscientist Luke Chang studies how

24:33 - 24:37

emotions like greed and guilt affect our decision-making.

24:37 - 24:39

MAN: Hey, Grace, we're going to start up the scout.

24:39 - 24:41

GRACE (on speaker): Okay.

24:41 - 24:43

Go ahead and make your decision.

24:44 - 24:45

(softly): Okay, did you tell her to go on to the next one?

24:45 - 24:46

MAN: Yep, you can hit next.

24:48 - 24:50

BERLIN: So, what are you guys looking at here?

24:50 - 24:52

What's this study about? Is there...

24:52 - 24:54

So she's playing an investment game...

24:54 - 24:55

Okay.

24:54 - 24:55

...with another participant,

24:55 - 24:57

who's outside the scanner.

24:57 - 25:01

BERLIN: Luke scans the brains of study participants

25:01 - 25:04

while they play a game from behavioral economics

25:04 - 25:06

called the Trust Game.

25:07 - 25:08

CHANG: This is a cooperative game

25:08 - 25:11

where one person has some sum of money,

25:11 - 25:13

and they can choose to invest any amount of that money

25:13 - 25:14

in their partner.

25:14 - 25:17

BERLIN: That investment grows.

25:17 - 25:22

So then, the study participant has to decide:

25:22 - 25:25

they could be greedy and keep all the money

25:25 - 25:27

or they could be generous,

25:27 - 25:29

and give some of the investment back.

25:29 - 25:31

♪ ♪

25:31 - 25:33

CHANG: We've always been really interested in,

25:33 - 25:36

why do people return the money when they don't have to?

25:36 - 25:39

And guilt provides one plausible mechanism

25:39 - 25:41

that might be driving their behavior

25:41 - 25:43

to act cooperatively in this game.

25:44 - 25:46

BERLIN: And so you're balancing making these decisions

25:46 - 25:50

between getting that kind of dopamine reward hit

25:50 - 25:51

from being a little selfish

25:52 - 25:56

versus being balanced by those feelings of, maybe, guilt

25:56 - 25:58

when you're not cooperating or helping somebody else out.

26:00 - 26:03

BERLIN: And the brain scans reveal which parts of the brain

26:03 - 26:07

are most active when someone is feeling guilt.

26:07 - 26:08

CHANG: Those regions ended up being

26:08 - 26:09

something called the insula.

26:09 - 26:12

Signals about having this gut feeling

26:12 - 26:14

that maybe this isn't a good idea,

26:14 - 26:16

or, "I'd feel really bad if I did that."

26:16 - 26:18

Those are the signals that originate from the insula

26:18 - 26:21

that allow us to make decisions to avoid harming someone else.

26:21 - 26:24

♪ ♪

26:24 - 26:26

BERLIN: Luke likes to think of it kind of like a thermometer

26:26 - 26:28

and a thermostat.

26:28 - 26:29

CHANG: If you try to think about how

26:29 - 26:31

a thermostat might be mapped onto the brain,

26:31 - 26:34

one region might be more like the thermometer,

26:34 - 26:37

detecting the ambient temperature in the room.

26:37 - 26:39

BERLIN: When it comes to reading the room,

26:39 - 26:43

our brain's thermometer seems to be the insula.

26:43 - 26:46

But all that information needs to go somewhere else

26:46 - 26:48

and be integrated with other types of information.

26:48 - 26:51

BERLIN: That's our brain's thermostat--

26:51 - 26:53

a region located inside the prefrontal cortex

26:53 - 26:58

that processes our emotions and helps regulate our behavior.

26:58 - 27:00

And while your thermostat can usually help you

27:00 - 27:02

take control of your emotions,

27:02 - 27:06

what would happen if it went out?

27:06 - 27:08

♪ ♪

27:08 - 27:11

CHANG: There's a famous patient named Phineas Gage.

27:11 - 27:13

WHEATLEY: Phineas Gage was a railroad foreman

27:13 - 27:15

who was working in Vermont,

27:15 - 27:21

and he was tamping down a hole that had gunpowder in it,

27:21 - 27:22

and the gunpowder ignited,

27:22 - 27:25

sending the rod through his eye, up through his brain,

27:25 - 27:30

taking out a big patch of his brain in the process.

27:30 - 27:33

At first people thought, well, this is a miracle.

27:33 - 27:36

This man has been unscathed from this accident.

27:36 - 27:41

He had memory, he had language, he had motor control.

27:41 - 27:43

But of course, his friends noticed a difference.

27:44 - 27:46

CHANG: His life fell apart-- he had a hard time holding a job,

27:46 - 27:52

he lost all of his friends, and he really just struggled.

27:52 - 27:54

WHEATLEY: His personality made him more fitful,

27:54 - 27:56

irreverent, more profane.

27:56 - 27:58

He was cursing a lot, lewd behavior.

27:58 - 28:00

So he had sort of no filter.

28:00 - 28:02

We now know that the parts of the brain

28:02 - 28:05

that he sort of surgically excised

28:05 - 28:09

were involved in emotion and control.

28:10 - 28:12

BERLIN: Over a hundred years later,

28:12 - 28:15

neuroscientists mapped the regions of his brain

28:15 - 28:18

that were harmed in that horrific accident.

28:18 - 28:20

Areas of his prefrontal cortex,

28:20 - 28:23

including the brain's thermostat, were damaged,

28:23 - 28:26

which might account for why he struggled socially.

28:26 - 28:28

He couldn't regulate his emotions

28:28 - 28:33

or process how other people might react to his behavior.

28:33 - 28:37

WHEATLEY: And that was the key moment, I think, in neuroscience history

28:37 - 28:39

when people realized,

28:39 - 28:42

oh, it's not just that motor, memory,

28:42 - 28:44

language is in the brain.

28:44 - 28:46

Your personality is up there, your morality is up there,

28:46 - 28:50

things that make you you are there.

28:50 - 28:54

BERLIN: I think we all kind of know intuitively that emotions

28:54 - 28:56

impact our decisions.

28:56 - 28:59

So what sort of extra information is this giving us?

28:59 - 29:01

CHANG: In a lot of the scientific work

29:01 - 29:03

that's been done on studying emotion in decision making,

29:03 - 29:05

people have really focused on

29:05 - 29:06

how emotions lead us to make worse decisions,

29:06 - 29:09

maybe even irrational.

29:09 - 29:11

And I actually don't think that's true.

29:11 - 29:13

If you have a goal to not want to harm others

29:13 - 29:16

and to do what's going to be in your self-interest,

29:16 - 29:19

emotions are actually helping us make better decisions.

29:19 - 29:21

♪ ♪

29:21 - 29:23

WHEATLEY: We are, in fact, the company that we keep,

29:23 - 29:27

because other people bring out parts of us,

29:27 - 29:29

and strengthen us in particular ways.

29:31 - 29:32

SCHILLER: How you make decisions,

29:32 - 29:35

how you behave, how you think about yourself,

29:35 - 29:37

all of these processes we develop

29:37 - 29:39

by mimicking and interacting

29:39 - 29:41

and synchronizing with other brains.

29:43 - 29:45

SAXE: One thing that we all share as humans

29:45 - 29:50

is that social life and social contact

29:50 - 29:53

is an incredibly important part of what our brain processes.

29:53 - 29:55

Our brains are, in detail, influenced

29:55 - 29:57

by every experience we have.

29:57 - 29:59

Every moment, every sentence, every image

29:59 - 30:03

changes your brain.

30:03 - 30:06

BERLIN: And certain experiences are so profound,

30:06 - 30:09

so extreme, that they can impact brain biology

30:09 - 30:13

from one generation to the next.

30:13 - 30:16

Neuroscientist Bianca Jones Marlin is studying

30:16 - 30:20

how your ancestors' experiences might control

30:20 - 30:22

how your brain is wired today.

30:22 - 30:25

MARLIN: We ask how trauma affects the brain,

30:25 - 30:27

how trauma affects the body,

30:27 - 30:31

and really, how trauma affects generations.

30:31 - 30:33

People in the world suffer from traumatic events,

30:34 - 30:35

and these traumatic events aren't just

30:35 - 30:38

a one-time change in their brain and their body.

30:38 - 30:40

It actually continues for seemingly their lifetime.

30:41 - 30:45

BERLIN: Bianca's research is inspired by her upbringing.

30:45 - 30:47

MARLIN: My parents, my biological parents,

30:47 - 30:49

were also foster parents.

30:49 - 30:51

So I had foster siblings and adopted siblings growing up.

30:51 - 30:54

Only now as a scientist, I realize that that motivates

30:54 - 30:56

a lot of the questions that I ask:

30:56 - 30:59

how do we understand what happens when kids

30:59 - 31:01

are born into trauma

31:01 - 31:03

and optimize what we do have for better generations?

31:03 - 31:06

♪ ♪

31:06 - 31:10

BERLIN: One insight comes from an event during World War II.

31:10 - 31:12

MARLIN: At the end of World War II,

31:12 - 31:15

the Netherlands were cut off from food by Nazi troops

31:15 - 31:17

because they decided to protest through the country.

31:17 - 31:20

And during this period of time, it created a man-made famine.

31:20 - 31:23

There was starvation, death, there was trauma.

31:23 - 31:25

BERLIN: Not only did those

31:25 - 31:28

who suffered during the famine experience health problems,

31:28 - 31:33

but some of their children, and even their grandchildren,

31:33 - 31:36

had metabolic issues.

31:36 - 31:38

So people began to ask, how does an experience

31:38 - 31:42

of a parent, of a grandparent, change offspring?

31:42 - 31:47

BERLIN: Researchers began to discover that your environment

31:47 - 31:50

and your experiences can change the way your genes

31:50 - 31:53

are activated in your body and in your brain.

31:53 - 31:55

MARLIN: It's not like you get your genes

31:56 - 31:57

and it's set in stone.

31:57 - 31:59

They're constantly changing based on the environment.

31:59 - 32:01

BERLIN: To see this in action,

32:01 - 32:03

Bianca studies mice.

32:03 - 32:06

MARLIN: We're able to map the whole genome of mice,

32:06 - 32:08

target certain areas

32:08 - 32:11

of that genetic code, and use them

32:11 - 32:13

to answer important questions in science.

32:13 - 32:16

BERLIN: So how could stress and trauma

32:16 - 32:19

alter the biology of the mice's offspring?

32:20 - 32:22

To find out,

32:22 - 32:24

Bianca paired the smell of almond

32:24 - 32:26

with an electric shock.

32:24 - 32:26

(shock buzzes)

32:26 - 32:29

MARLIN: Because mice really navigate the world

32:29 - 32:31

and rely heavily on the sense of smell,

32:31 - 32:34

we use olfaction, pair it with a light foot shock,

32:34 - 32:37

and we observe changes in the brain and changes in behavior.

32:38 - 32:39

BERLIN: She noticed that something inside

32:39 - 32:44

of the mice's noses changed.

32:44 - 32:46

MARLIN: We're able to look at the cells in the nose

32:46 - 32:47

that only respond to almond.

32:48 - 32:51

And what we observe is that after the light foot shock

32:51 - 32:54

and the presentation of almond coinciding,

32:54 - 32:55

there are more cells in the nose

32:55 - 32:57

that express the almond receptor.

32:57 - 33:00

It's as if something in the milieu of the nose says,

33:00 - 33:02

almond's important in this environment.

33:02 - 33:03

We need more cells like you.

33:04 - 33:06

BERLIN: Mice grew more cells

33:06 - 33:09

that responded to the smell of almond.

33:09 - 33:11

MARLIN: Each one of these green dots you see here,

33:12 - 33:13

these are neurons.

33:13 - 33:16

They're cells that can respond to the almond smell.

33:17 - 33:20

These red dots are cells that were born

33:20 - 33:22

after the presentation of odor and shock.

33:23 - 33:27

And this cell right here, this red and green cell,

33:27 - 33:28

is a cell that was born

33:28 - 33:30

after the presentation of almond and shock

33:30 - 33:33

that also responds to almond.

33:33 - 33:35

This is the cell that we want to look at

33:35 - 33:37

to see what information is inside,

33:37 - 33:38

because we see more of these

33:39 - 33:40

after the odor and shock pairing.

33:41 - 33:44

BERLIN: Remarkably, these changes were actually passed down

33:44 - 33:48

to the next generation.

33:48 - 33:50

MARLIN: The offspring, the kids of the parents

33:50 - 33:53

that were shocked with odor,

33:53 - 33:57

were born with more cells that express the almond receptor.

33:57 - 33:58

Which means there's a memory that somehow

33:58 - 34:02

is maintained in sperm and egg through implantation

34:02 - 34:05

and represented in offspring.

34:05 - 34:09

It is as if we are observing a change in evolution

34:09 - 34:14

over the time span of one generation.

34:14 - 34:16

And I just think that's fascinating.

34:16 - 34:19

Because we as humans know how

34:19 - 34:23

environment and how traumatic events change people.

34:24 - 34:27

Just being able to take the science of that

34:27 - 34:29

and being able to show that,

34:29 - 34:32

we're just justifying what we already know as humans,

34:32 - 34:33

what society has known for a long time,

34:33 - 34:35

what individuals know.

34:35 - 34:39

We just want to bring that to an undeniable truth.

34:41 - 34:43

MARTINEZ-CONDE: Our brains are not static.

34:43 - 34:47

We try to make sense of what's happening right now,

34:47 - 34:52

but we also try to make sense of what happened a long time ago

34:52 - 34:54

and to have, like, this grand picture

34:54 - 34:56

of our life as a trajectory.

34:58 - 35:00

Our ability for conscious awareness.

35:00 - 35:01

It's a magnificent ability, this ability

35:01 - 35:03

to reflect on our own minds.

35:03 - 35:06

But it also leads us astray.

35:06 - 35:08

SETH: I have memories, plans,

35:08 - 35:12

I have these feelings of agency over my actions.

35:12 - 35:14

But what the science itself is telling us is that

35:14 - 35:17

these things aren't necessarily bound together.

35:17 - 35:19

Different aspects of the self can be manipulated,

35:19 - 35:21

or even taken away altogether.

35:22 - 35:25

BERLIN: Your biology and the choices you make

35:25 - 35:27

are all molded by your social interactions

35:27 - 35:30

and even your family history.

35:30 - 35:33

And yet, we feel like we have control.

35:33 - 35:37

Like we have agency, right?

35:37 - 35:41

♪ ♪

35:43 - 35:48

MAOZ: An agent is somebody that is the author of their own story.

35:49 - 35:51

But actually,

35:51 - 35:53

most of what's happening in our brain we are not conscious of.

35:53 - 35:56

And I think this gets you starting to think,

35:56 - 35:58

wait a minute, you know,

35:58 - 36:00

is really everything under my control?

36:02 - 36:03

BERLIN: Neuroscientist Uri Maoz

36:03 - 36:06

is putting our sense of control to the test.

36:08 - 36:09

We feel like we're in control,

36:09 - 36:11

but where exactly does that

36:11 - 36:14

feeling come from, and how does it work?

36:14 - 36:17

Ah, here you are.

36:17 - 36:19

Hello.

36:17 - 36:19

Hello.

36:19 - 36:21

Thank you very much for joining us,

36:21 - 36:24

agent-ically and out of your own volition.

36:24 - 36:26

(laughs): Of course.

36:24 - 36:26

Before we start...

36:26 - 36:28

Mm-hmm.

36:26 - 36:28

...let me give you this envelope.

36:28 - 36:31

Okay.

36:28 - 36:31

Please don't let anybody touch it.

36:31 - 36:34

Okay.

36:31 - 36:34

And don't look inside, but we'll need it for later on.

36:34 - 36:35

For later, okay.

36:35 - 36:37

BERLIN: To show me how my sense of control

36:37 - 36:38

isn't always what it seems,

36:39 - 36:41

Uri kicked things off by trying to get me

36:41 - 36:46

to question my ability to choose by using a magic trick.

36:46 - 36:48

So where would you like to sit?

36:48 - 36:49

Where would I like to sit?

36:49 - 36:51

MAOZ: It's really up to you.

36:49 - 36:51

BERLIN: It's really-- I have a choice?

36:51 - 36:53

MAOZ: Wherever you want-- you have a choice.

36:53 - 36:54

All right, so I'm going to sit here.

36:55 - 36:56

You're going to sit over there, okay.

36:55 - 36:56

Yes.

36:56 - 36:58

So how about just before you sit down, if you don't mind...

36:58 - 36:59

Mm-hmm.

36:59 - 37:03

Um, let's see what this says.

37:03 - 37:05

Oh, my God, okay.

37:03 - 37:05

So...

37:05 - 37:09

So then that one obviously says the same thing, right?

37:09 - 37:11

Um...

37:09 - 37:11

No?

37:11 - 37:13

Let's check and see what this one says.

37:13 - 37:16

This one says...

37:13 - 37:16

Oh, come on.

37:16 - 37:19

Okay.

37:16 - 37:19

(laughs): So I'm that predictable?

37:19 - 37:21

You don't even know me yet!

37:22 - 37:24

BERLIN: I really don't know how he did that!

37:24 - 37:29

I'm not totally convinced, but I'm starting to question,

37:29 - 37:31

how do I know when I have made a decision?

37:31 - 37:34

If I may, let me give you,

37:34 - 37:35

as a present, a book.

37:35 - 37:37

Here you go, this is yours.

37:35 - 37:37

Oh, thank you.

37:37 - 37:39

And I will just ask you to leaf through it...

37:39 - 37:42

Mm-hmm.

37:39 - 37:42

...and find a word that has some meaning for you.

37:42 - 37:44

All right, I got it.

37:44 - 37:45

Can you tell me what the word is?

37:45 - 37:47

Representation.

37:47 - 37:49

Please write the word down, representation.

37:47 - 37:49

Mm-hmm.

37:49 - 37:51

And, you know, just stick that sticky note

37:51 - 37:53

somewhere on that page, yeah, thank you.

37:51 - 37:53

Okay, okay, all right.

37:53 - 37:55

BERLIN: We'll come back to that later.

37:56 - 37:59

But for now, I'm starting to see how choice

37:59 - 38:02

and agency aren't always so straightforward.

38:03 - 38:06

So to find out what's actually going on in the brain

38:06 - 38:08

when our sense of control is in question,

38:08 - 38:10

I took a look at a trial designed

38:10 - 38:13

by post-doctoral researcher Alice Wong.

38:13 - 38:17

A volunteer from the lab, Tomás, is being fitted

38:17 - 38:21

with a transcranial magnetic stimulation device,

38:21 - 38:24

TMS for short.

38:24 - 38:27

It generates a strong magnetic field

38:27 - 38:29

that can send signals to your brain.

38:29 - 38:30

MAOZ: The idea is that you

38:30 - 38:34

stimulate the brain using a focused magnetic field.

38:34 - 38:37

And if you stimulate that in the right part

38:37 - 38:41

of the motor cortex-- it's a part of the brain

38:41 - 38:43

that actually controls your fingers--

38:43 - 38:44

it's like you're pulling on a string here.

38:44 - 38:46

Every time you pull it, the finger goes.

38:48 - 38:50

BERLIN: With the device hooked up,

38:50 - 38:52

the researchers can make his finger jump

38:52 - 38:56

involuntarily by sending a signal to his motor cortex.

38:56 - 38:59

(device clicks)

38:56 - 38:59

WONG: We're going to be locating

38:59 - 39:03

the spot of your motor cortex that moves one of your fingers.

39:04 - 39:05

(device clicks)

39:05 - 39:06

How about that?

39:05 - 39:06

TOMÁS: That works.

39:06 - 39:09

That was a pinky movement up.

39:06 - 39:09

WONG: Okay.

39:10 - 39:14

BERLIN: Sometimes they ask him to move his finger on his own.

39:14 - 39:17

WONG: Could you replicate the movement if in, that you...

39:17 - 39:19

TOMÁS: Was something like this.

39:19 - 39:21

BERLIN: Remarkably, by recording

39:21 - 39:24

the small electrical signals that travel from his brain

39:24 - 39:26

down to his finger muscles,

39:26 - 39:29

Alice and Uri can pinpoint the exact moment

39:29 - 39:32

that Tomás's brain has initiated a movement--

39:32 - 39:36

almost 50 milliseconds before he actually moves.

39:36 - 39:41

With this information, it's as though they can predict

39:41 - 39:44

his movement slightly before it actually happens.

39:44 - 39:49

So now, his sense of agency is about to be put to the test.

39:49 - 39:50

WONG: Who initiated the movement?

39:50 - 39:52

TOMÁS: It was me.

39:52 - 39:54

WONG: How much agency did you feel over the movement?

39:54 - 39:55

TOMÁS: Quite a lot.

39:55 - 39:57

Full agency? Okay.

39:57 - 40:00

BERLIN: Normally, the researcher isn't in the room,

40:00 - 40:04

and all the questions are conducted by the computer.

40:04 - 40:06

Who initiated the movement?

40:08 - 40:11

I don't know.

40:11 - 40:14

How much agency did you feel over the movement?

40:15 - 40:17

TOMÁS: I would say some agency.

40:17 - 40:18

BERLIN: In some instances,

40:18 - 40:23

just as Tomás decides to move his finger,

40:23 - 40:25

the researchers use the magnetic field to make his finger move.

40:25 - 40:27

(device clicks)

40:25 - 40:27

WONG: Who initiated the movement?

40:27 - 40:30

I really don't know.

40:30 - 40:32

Okay.

40:32 - 40:35

How much agency did you feel over the movement?

40:35 - 40:37

A little bit.

40:37 - 40:39

BERLIN: So, even in the instances

40:39 - 40:42

when Tomás really did decide to move his finger...

40:42 - 40:44

WONG: How much agency did you feel

40:44 - 40:47

over the movement?

40:44 - 40:47

No agency at all.

40:47 - 40:50

BERLIN: ...he didn't always feel like he was in control.

40:50 - 40:54

So after the experiment, I was excited to hear the results.

40:54 - 40:59

MAOZ: When Tomás initiated the movement himself,

40:59 - 41:02

yet we intervened with the TMS,

41:02 - 41:06

Tomás said, "That wasn't me, I didn't initiate the movement.

41:06 - 41:09

It was the computer."

41:09 - 41:11

He thought that the computer initiated the movement,

41:11 - 41:12

or it was both of them, or he wasn't sure,

41:12 - 41:14

but he almost never said that it was him.

41:15 - 41:17

BERLIN: So what do you think is going on there?

41:17 - 41:19

How is this happening?

41:19 - 41:21

MAOZ: You know, we walk around and we feel like, you know,

41:21 - 41:23

we are the authors of our, of our actions and so on.

41:23 - 41:25

And you can see with just a little bit of messing around,

41:26 - 41:27

it tends to fall apart.

41:27 - 41:30

BERLIN: It's fragile, like our sense of self...

41:27 - 41:30

MAOZ: Yes.

41:30 - 41:32

BERLIN: ...our memories, our sense of agency.

41:32 - 41:35

They're all things that our brain evolved over time.

41:35 - 41:38

BERLIN: But they're fragile and they can be manipulated...

41:35 - 41:38

MAOZ: Yes.

41:38 - 41:39

BERLIN: ...under the right circumstances.

41:39 - 41:41

MAOZ: Everything has to align

41:41 - 41:42

for you to feel the sense of agency.

41:42 - 41:44

When the finger moves,

41:44 - 41:46

we get this feedback back to the brain

41:46 - 41:48

and it's incorporated with whatever is happening

41:48 - 41:49

in the brain to create the movement.

41:49 - 41:53

MAOZ: And together you get this sense of agency over the movement.

41:53 - 41:57

I think that in everyday life, we are in control.

41:57 - 42:00

However, I think this experiment shows we're quite happy

42:00 - 42:02

to relinquish control.

42:02 - 42:04

BERLIN: Like states of consciousness,

42:04 - 42:05

there are levels of agency,

42:05 - 42:10

ways it can be manipulated, and even taken away.

42:10 - 42:13

We think A happened and then B happened.

42:13 - 42:15

That's the end of the story.

42:15 - 42:18

But of course, most of our brain activity is unconscious.

42:18 - 42:20

Who initiated the first movement?

42:20 - 42:23

That was me.

42:23 - 42:24

SETH: So, we sometimes misinterpret.

42:24 - 42:26

Our experience of voluntary action

42:26 - 42:28

is a little bit retrospective in this sense.

42:28 - 42:30

The brain looks at what the body did,

42:30 - 42:32

and figures out if that makes sense

42:32 - 42:35

as an act of its own free will.

42:35 - 42:36

♪ ♪

42:36 - 42:39

BERLIN: After the agency experiment,

42:39 - 42:41

we had more important matters to attend to.

42:41 - 42:45

So, Heather, when you came in, I gave you an envelope, right?

42:45 - 42:47

Yes.

42:45 - 42:47

Nobody touched it but you?

42:47 - 42:48

No.

42:48 - 42:51

Do you remember that later on,

42:51 - 42:52

I gave you that book?

42:51 - 42:52

Mm-hmm.

42:53 - 42:54

And in that book, you opened it

42:54 - 42:58

to whatever page you wanted, and you found a word in there.

42:54 - 42:58

Mm-hmm.

42:58 - 43:00

Right, where the...

42:58 - 43:00

Can you tell us again what that word was?

43:00 - 43:02

Yes, it was on page 105.

43:02 - 43:05

And the word was "representation."

43:05 - 43:07

Representation, okay.

43:07 - 43:10

So if you don't mind just putting the book aside

43:10 - 43:12

and if you could take the envelope out now.

43:10 - 43:12

Okay.

43:12 - 43:14

Can you open it and see what's inside, please?

43:14 - 43:15

Oh, this is one of these things

43:16 - 43:18

that's gonna freak me out, right?

43:16 - 43:18

Let's see.

43:18 - 43:19

I'm getting chills.

43:24 - 43:27

Come on. No way!

43:27 - 43:30

Come on-- no, seriously!

43:27 - 43:30

(both laugh)

43:30 - 43:32

That's really freaky.

43:32 - 43:34

So you're in control, right?

43:34 - 43:37

I don't know how you did that-- that is really weird.

43:37 - 43:38

I mean, what do I do now?

43:38 - 43:41

(laughs): I don't know where to-- what do I do with that?

43:41 - 43:43

BERLIN: Uri's magic acts are tricks.

43:43 - 43:46

Sleights-of-hand and misdirection.

43:46 - 43:48

But when I saw what was written on the card,

43:48 - 43:52

I have to admit I wondered if my choices mattered at all.

43:52 - 43:55

Going to do this...

43:55 - 43:56

BERLIN: Alice Wong's experiment supports

43:56 - 43:59

the idea that it isn't just about what happens in the brain

43:59 - 44:01

at the moment a decision is made.

44:01 - 44:03

How did you do that?

44:03 - 44:05

BERLIN: Your sense of agency or control

44:05 - 44:09

also has to do with feedback you get after the decision--

44:09 - 44:12

physical, social, and emotional.

44:12 - 44:15

I think of agency as a sense, so there is a sense of agency

44:15 - 44:18

that sometimes can get disrupted, perhaps,

44:18 - 44:20

just like you have a sense of sight or smell and so on.

44:20 - 44:22

Sometimes, you have visual illusions.

44:22 - 44:24

It's similar with a sense of agency.

44:24 - 44:27

I can manipulate your sense of agency.

44:27 - 44:28

But that doesn't mean that we never

44:28 - 44:30

have a sense of agency.

44:30 - 44:32

♪ ♪

44:32 - 44:34

BERLIN: Your brain is a meaning-maker machine.

44:34 - 44:37

And creating a sense of agency

44:37 - 44:40

is one of the ways it makes meaning out of your daily life.

44:42 - 44:45

BANAJI: There is no way in which I can operate

44:45 - 44:46

without understanding

44:46 - 44:49

what is happening and why I'm doing it.

44:49 - 44:53

It's the filling-in of the blanks that is necessary

44:53 - 44:55

in some ways for survival, to give meaning, to make sense

44:55 - 44:58

of the cause and effect of things.

44:58 - 45:00

KASTHURI: Perhaps we have that feeling of consciousness

45:00 - 45:03

because it gives me a sense of agency.

45:03 - 45:07

It allows me to pretend like I'm the one making decisions

45:07 - 45:08

and I'm the one reaping the rewards

45:09 - 45:12

or the failures of that particular decision.

45:12 - 45:15

BERLIN: There are parts of the brain that allow you

45:15 - 45:18

to feel like the author of your own life.

45:18 - 45:20

But that's only part of the story.

45:20 - 45:22

(echoing): Each of our minds is actually composed

45:22 - 45:24

of lots of different pieces that are doing different things.

45:24 - 45:27

This illusion that there's a single person

45:27 - 45:29

inside our skulls.

45:29 - 45:31

MARLIN: We know how environment

45:31 - 45:34

and how traumatic events change people.

45:34 - 45:36

Our brains are, in detail, influenced

45:36 - 45:39

by the expectations of the people around us.

45:39 - 45:44

But of course, most of our brain activity is unconscious.

45:44 - 45:46

(playing slow tune)

45:46 - 45:49

BERLIN: But there are some situations where letting go

45:49 - 45:52

of conscious control can have amazing results.

45:55 - 45:57

LIMB: When you're playing the blues,

45:57 - 45:59

you have this kind of well-known musical structure,

45:59 - 46:01

this template, and then you use that

46:01 - 46:02

as a launchpad for improvisation,

46:02 - 46:05

for innovation, and for new ideas.

46:05 - 46:08

BERLIN: Charles Limb is a neuroscientist

46:08 - 46:11

trying to understand how our brain operates

46:11 - 46:13

when we are being truly creative.

46:13 - 46:16

CHRIS EMDIN: ♪ It's gon' be ill in the MRI ♪

46:16 - 46:18

BERLIN: And today,

46:18 - 46:20

he's using a scanner to peer into the brain

46:20 - 46:23

of educator and freestyle rapper Chris Emdin.

46:23 - 46:26

♪ I wonder if I'm going insane as I'm freestyling, profiling ♪

46:26 - 46:28

♪ Still wilin', it's gon' be ill ♪

46:28 - 46:30

You ready for me?

46:28 - 46:30

WOMAN: Yes.

46:33 - 46:35

LIMB: Okay, remember, keep your head still

46:35 - 46:38

during the entire thing and try not to move your feet

46:38 - 46:40

or your hands at all during the rapping.

46:40 - 46:41

EMDIN (on speaker): Okay, doing the best I can.

46:41 - 46:43

Yeah, awesome, thank you.

46:43 - 46:48

BERLIN: First, Charles asks Chris to perform a memorized piece.

46:48 - 46:50

Now, that memory means you're going to do the memorized lyrics

46:50 - 46:52

the way you originally wrote them.

46:52 - 46:55

Okay?

46:52 - 46:55

EMDIN: Okay.

46:55 - 46:56

LIMB: Memory.

46:56 - 46:59

EMDIN: ♪ I'm a physicist, lyricist, spitting this ridiculousness ♪

46:59 - 47:01

♪ So witness the ignorance I dismiss ♪

46:59 - 47:01

Up a little bit?

47:01 - 47:03

♪ Feelings and emotion is the topic of the course ♪

47:03 - 47:06

♪ Staying motionless to handle balanced force ♪

47:06 - 47:09

BERLIN: Next, he gives him a prompt and asks him

47:09 - 47:14

to improvise-- to create a new, original piece on the spot.

47:14 - 47:16

He doesn't know what's coming.

47:14 - 47:16

Mm-hmm.

47:16 - 47:18

And that's going to be his cues for that.

47:18 - 47:20

LIMB: Freestyle: physicist.

47:20 - 47:23

EMDIN: ♪ Physicist, lyricist ♪

47:23 - 47:25

♪ Emcees like this will always be kicking this ♪

47:25 - 47:26

♪ After all of that it'll all be over ♪

47:26 - 47:30

♪ Lucky like I picked a four-leaf clover ♪

47:30 - 47:32

♪ Can't move my shoulder ♪

47:32 - 47:34

♪ 'Cause the MRI machine won't let me do it ♪

47:34 - 47:36

♪ But you wouldn't know what it is that it's like ♪

47:34 - 47:36

(laughs)

47:36 - 47:39

♪ I'm like a baseball player the way I strike ♪

47:39 - 47:41

♪ With the raps... ♪

47:39 - 47:41

LIMB: Stop.

47:41 - 47:42

(chuckles): He's good.

47:44 - 47:47

BERLIN: So, what does improvisation or spontaneous creativity

47:47 - 47:50

look like in the brain?

47:50 - 47:51

LIMB: What we found was that

47:51 - 47:55

the prefrontal cortex that appears to be linked

47:55 - 47:58

to effortful self-monitoring

47:58 - 48:00

seemed to be turning off,

48:00 - 48:03

deactivating, in a pretty intense way

48:03 - 48:05

in these highly trained professional musicians

48:05 - 48:07

when they start improvising.

48:07 - 48:12

So in some sense, by letting go, by decreasing activation

48:12 - 48:14

in the prefrontal cortex,

48:14 - 48:17

we can sort of gain control of our lives in a way.

48:17 - 48:19

LIMB: In fact, if you're too self-conscious

48:19 - 48:21

and you're unable to relax and let go,

48:21 - 48:23

you can't do something like this.

48:23 - 48:26

When you start trying to put conscious control mechanism,

48:26 - 48:27

your performance goes down-- you get worse.

48:27 - 48:29

So would you say this goes to, to any activity,

48:29 - 48:32

really, if you're, for a professional tennis player

48:32 - 48:34

or if you're trying to do a physical activity,

48:34 - 48:37

that the more you're able to practice letting go,

48:37 - 48:40

once you've learnt the skill, the better you'll be.

48:37 - 48:40

Exactly.

48:40 - 48:44

LIMB: Free throw shooters that are able to shoot 99% free throws,

48:44 - 48:46

all of a sudden, when you tell them

48:46 - 48:48

you're going to get a million dollars

48:48 - 48:49

if you make the next one...

48:48 - 48:49

Mm-hmm.

48:49 - 48:52

Then all of a sudden, you inject conscious control over something

48:52 - 48:54

that's much better just to left to its own subconsciousness.

48:54 - 48:56

And then your performance gets worse,

48:56 - 48:57

and you're more likely to choke.

48:57 - 49:00

BERLIN: Surprisingly, the parts of your brain

49:00 - 49:04

that are usually in control can get in your way.

49:04 - 49:07

Your prefrontal cortex, the decision maker,

49:07 - 49:11

can make you overthink something you've done a thousand times.

49:13 - 49:15

LIMB: Freestyle: stay.

49:15 - 49:17

EMDIN: ♪ Yes, you want me to stay ♪

49:17 - 49:19

♪ Relaxed, but I won't never play ♪

49:19 - 49:23

LIMB: Every human being is creative.

49:23 - 49:24

Whether they're creative artistically or not

49:24 - 49:27

is another question, but we're all creative.

49:27 - 49:29

We have to be, because all day long, we're doing

49:29 - 49:31

unscripted things that we didn't know we would be doing.

49:31 - 49:34

Life is not scripted.

49:34 - 49:35

And so no matter who you are

49:35 - 49:39

in this world, you're doing things that are unplanned.

49:40 - 49:42

BERLIN: All day long, we're balancing forces

49:42 - 49:45

that push us around, even if we're not aware of them,

49:45 - 49:49

from past trauma to the emotions of others,

49:49 - 49:52

and all the hidden forces affecting your brain.

49:52 - 49:55

KASTHURI: I'd like to believe that I am in charge of my life,

49:55 - 49:58

that I am the agent of my life,

49:58 - 50:00

that I actually can control my emotions,

50:01 - 50:03

my abilities, my desires.

50:03 - 50:05

And the more I learn about brains, the more I realize

50:05 - 50:07

that this is probably not true.

50:08 - 50:10

SETH: We can be influenced by our social networks,

50:10 - 50:13

by our culture, by our genetics,

50:13 - 50:17

by our development, by our childhood.

50:17 - 50:19

(clock ticking)

50:19 - 50:21

BERLIN: Your brain is a complicated collection

50:21 - 50:24

of these intricate parts,

50:24 - 50:26

many of which you have no awareness of,

50:26 - 50:29

and they all work together in a delicate dance

50:29 - 50:33

to create your perception of you.

50:33 - 50:36

KANWISHER: The brain is who you are.

50:36 - 50:38

It's really different than any other organ in that sense.

50:39 - 50:41

MARTINEZ-CONDE: We know that every experience,

50:41 - 50:44

every thought, every memory,

50:44 - 50:48

every sensation has its origin in the brain.

50:49 - 50:52

KASTHURI: The brain is made of almost 90 billion neurons,

50:52 - 50:53

but it produces the idea

50:53 - 50:56

that there's a single thing inside my head.

50:56 - 50:58

My particular pattern of neuronal connections,

50:58 - 51:00

it actually creates me.

51:00 - 51:02

And your particular pattern of neuronal connections

51:02 - 51:06

actually creates you.

51:06 - 51:09

BERLIN: Years of studying the brain have humbled me.

51:09 - 51:11

BERLIN: He looks scared.

51:11 - 51:13

BERLIN: You can't control everything

51:13 - 51:16

that makes you who you are.

51:16 - 51:20

But the unconscious you is still you.

51:20 - 51:23

BANAJI: The vast majority

51:23 - 51:27

of the brain's work is happening outside conscious awareness.

51:27 - 51:29

(crowd groans)

51:27 - 51:29

LIMB: If you try to over-control some things,

51:29 - 51:31

you actually will decrease your performance.

51:31 - 51:34

LIMB: You have to let go

51:34 - 51:36

of conscious self-monitoring to just kind of, like,

51:36 - 51:38

go with the flow.

51:38 - 51:40

It could be scary to say and scary to hear,

51:40 - 51:42

but we are not just our own.

51:43 - 51:46

WHEATLEY: We are all multifaceted, multi-dimensional people.

51:47 - 51:49

BERLIN: And by becoming more aware

51:49 - 51:53

of the unconscious processes in your own brain,

51:53 - 51:55

you can become more aware of what drives you,

51:55 - 52:00

and what you ultimately can control.

52:13 - 52:21

♪ ♪

52:25 - 52:32

♪ ♪

52:34 - 52:41

♪ ♪

52:43 - 52:51

♪ ♪

52:56 - 53:04

♪ ♪

The Intriguing Mystery of the Human Brain

Have you ever felt like you were in complete control of your decisions, only to find yourself second-guessing what happened next? The brain is an intricate three-pound matter that dictates everything we do, from conscious decisions to unconscious behaviors. "Your Brain: Who's In Control?" explores the fascinating interplay between conscious awareness and unconscious processes within our brains.

Unmasking the Unconscious Brain

Neuroscientists like Charles Limb and Uri Maoz delve deep into understanding how our brains function without our conscious awareness. Through experiments on sleepwalkers, split-brain surgery recipients, and patients under anesthesia, these researchers uncover how different brain regions operate independently, sometimes leading to surprising outcomes.

The Influence of Trauma and Genetics

Bianca Jones Marlin investigates how traumatic events can leave generational imprints on brain biology. Her studies with mice reveal how trauma experienced by ancestors can manifest in the brain structure of offspring. These findings shed light on how external influences shape our brains and behaviors.

Agency and Creative Control

Discover how our brains handle the complexities of decision-making and creativity. From the delicate balance of agency in the brain to the freedom of improvisation in musicians, our sense of control is far more nuanced than we might think. The brain's ability to let go of conscious control can lead to incredible bouts of creativity and performance.

Emotions, Social Interactions, and Self-Reflection

Explore how emotions, social connections, and self-awareness play pivotal roles in shaping our brains and behaviors. Researchers like Luke Chang and Michael Gazzaniga delve into how our interactions with others and our ability to reflect on ourselves influence our brain activities and decision-making processes.

The Brain: A Multifaceted Puzzle

While the brain remains the biggest mystery in science, unraveling its complexities helps us understand more about our very being. The brain's unconscious processes, intertwined with conscious decisions, paint a rich tapestry of human behavior and identity. So, next time you feel like you're in control, remember, there's more to your brain than meets the eye.

In the ever-evolving landscape of neuroscience, every discovery unveils a new layer of the intricate puzzle that is the human brain. Our consciousness, emotions, and behaviors are but the tip of the iceberg in the vast realm of neurological exploration. As we journey deeper into the enigmatic realms of the brain, we unveil more about what truly makes us who we are.